Добавил:
Опубликованный материал нарушает ваши авторские права? Сообщите нам.
Вуз: Предмет: Файл:

Книги по МРТ КТ на английском языке / Functional Neuroimaging in Child Psychiatry Ernst 1 ed 2000

.pdf
Скачиваний:
2
Добавлен:
05.10.2023
Размер:
11.26 Mб
Скачать

374A. C. H. Lee et al.

DNMTS task performance in monkeys depends on intact ventromedial prefrontal function (see Murray, 1992).

A variation of this task has been used in a PET study by Elliott and Dolan (1998) in an eVort to investigate the neural network underlying short-term visual memory. The underlying experimental paradigm was that subjects were presented with a test stimulus composed of four subelements and then, after a short delay of a few seconds, four choice stimuli made of similar subelements. Depending on the condition, the subelements of the choice stimulus were either the same shape as those of the test stimulus but of a diVerent color (color-only condition), the same color as those of the test stimulus but of a diVerent shape (shapeonly condition) or, a mixture of these two (conjunction condition). The subjects had to remember and recall the test stimulus on the basis of either the colors of the subelements, the shape, or both the color and the shape of the subelements. The control task was designed to match the perceptual and motor demands of the experimental tasks but lacked a visual memory component. Subjects were presented with four choice stimuli, which were also made up of four subelements but did not bear any resemblance to the preceding test stimuli. The subjects were explicitly instructed not to remember the test stimuli and to respond to the four choice stimuli by simply pressing a prespeciWed response button.

By subtracting the perceptuomotor control from all of the memory conditions combined, Elliott and Dolan (1998) were able to isolate regions speciWc to short-term visual memory. These regions included the extrastriate cortex, medial and lateral parietal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, inferior frontal cortex, and the thalamus. In contrast to many of the tests reviewed above, the DMTS paradigm did not produce a widespread pattern of activation in the prefrontal cortex, congruent with the relative lack of eVects of frontal lobe excisions on this task (Owen et al., 1995a). Subtracting the color-only and shape-only conditions from each other showed that these conditions were associated with subtly diVerent activation patterns. However, the conjunction condition was found not to activate any regions that were not already active in the coloronly or shape-only condition. This Wnding suggests that, at least in short-term visual memory, there are no speciWc cortical regions that are responsible for remembering perceptual conjunctions between features. Rather, there may be speciWc posterior areas that are responsible for remembering speciWc types of feature. From their observations, Elliott and Dolan (1998) suggest that the left lingual gyrus may be involved in the memory of color whereas the medial occipital gyrus, right inferior parietal cortex and

precuneus may be speciWcally associated with memory for shape.

Summary and future directions

The use of functional neuroimaging has greatly increased the power of the CANTAB by enabling the deWnition of speciWc neural networks that are necessary for the adequate performance of the diVerent tasks (Fig. 21.2). These data help to validate the neural assumptions of the battery, which are based on studies of humans or monkeys with brain lesions, and also help to isolate discrete cognitive components of the tests, with consequent implications for theories of their underlying cognitive processes. We have also illustrated the use of the tests for analyzing further the nature of the cognitive deWcits exhibited in disorders such as depression or Parkinson's disease, a list we can expect to grow longer and to include neurodevelopmental disorders. The particular attractions of CANTAB for this purpose includes (i) its relationship to the existing animal neuropsychologic literature; (ii) its lack of dependence on language functions; and (iii) its componential nature, which is just as well suited to the gradual development of cognitive function as its gradual decline (as occurs in the dementias, which provided the initial impetus for the construction of the battery).

Whereas the H215O PET method is powerful, it is likely that it will be supplanted, or at any rate augmented, by alternative neuroimaging methods with greater temporal or chemical speciWcity. Reference was made to the possibility of using fMRI to resolve some interesting issues for the attentional set-shifting task. So far, this method with its improved temporal resolution has not often been used with the CANTAB; however we expect this to change in the near future in cognitive activation studies, especially with the better prospects oVered for imaging children using fMRI than PET (because of the ethical constraints imposed by the latter, see Chapter 2). It is useful to note that there has been a recent study of the ªone touchº Tower of London task using fMRI that largely con®rmed the results obtained using PET reviewed above (Baker et al., 1996). The ªeasyº and ªdiYcultº conditions both produced a signi®cant activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Dorso±ventral and anterior±posterior extensions of these activations were associated with the increased working memory load involved in planning more diYcult solutions (Granon et al., 1998).

It has also proved possible recently to correlate the performance of several of the CANTAB tests, including the Tower of London and spatial span, with levels of dopamine

The utility of CANTAB in functional imaging

375

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Parietal lobe:

Parietal lobe:

 

 

 

 

Premotor

CAN. ToL

CAN. ToL; RVIP;

Premotor cortex:

Dorsolateral

cortex: 1T ToL;

RVIP; DMTS

DMTS

CAN. ToL; EDS

 

 

 

1T ToL; CAN. ToL

PFC: 1T ToL;

Dorsolateral

 

Occipitoparietal

Occipitoparietal

 

 

 

SWM; SS;

PFC: Diff. CAN.

 

cortex: 1T ToL

cortex: 1T ToL

 

 

 

EDS; RVIP

ToL; 1T ToL; RVIP

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Visual

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

cortex:

Visual

 

 

 

 

 

 

DMTS;

cortex:

 

 

 

 

 

 

Diff. CAN. ToL

DMTS

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Occipital

 

 

 

 

Frontal pole:

 

Occipital lobe:

 

 

 

 

EDS

 

lobe: CAN. ToL;

 

 

 

 

 

 

CAN. ToL; RVIP

RVIP

Fusiform gyrus:

Ventrolateral

Ventrolateral

Fusiform gyrus:

 

RVIP

 

PFC: Diff. CAN. ToL;

PFC: Diff. CAN. ToL;

RVIP

 

 

 

 

 

 

SS; RVIP; DMTS

RVIP; DMTS

 

 

 

Right hemisphere

 

 

Left hemisphere

Fig. 21.2. Summary of the cerebral cortical locations of the main activations described for the CANTAB tests described in this chapter. RVIP, rapid visual information processing; SWM, spatial working memory; SS, spatial span; DMTS, delayed-matching-to-sample; CAN. ToL, CANTAB version of the Tower of London; DiV. CAN. ToL, diYcult problems (four or more moves) of CAN. ToL; IT ToL, one-touch Tower of London; EDS, extradimensional shift on the attentional set-shifting paradigm; PFC, prefrontal cortex (see text for further details).

D1 and D2 receptor ligands in the caudate and putamen of patients with Huntington's disease (Lawrence et al., 1998b), suggesting an approach to understanding the neurochemical basis of performance on some of the tasks. The approach here was one of straightforward correlation of performances on several neuropsychologic tests, including those from CANTAB, measured outside the scanner with indices of ligand binding obtained via PET. This might be expanded still further in light of the exciting recent demonstration of displacement of a dopamine D2 receptor ligand from binding sites in the ventral striatum by performance of a video game by healthy men (Koepp et al., 1998). A complementary approach is suggested by the results of several recent psychopharmacologic investigations of tests from CANTAB, including eVects of adrenergic (clonidine; Coull et al., 1995), cholinergic (scopolamine (hyoscine); Robbins et al., 1997), and dopaminergic agents (methylphenidate, Elliott et al., 1997c). It would obviously be of interest to locate the speciWc sites at which these drugs are exerting their eVects by observing changes in rCBF that correlate with the drug eVect. With the availability of speciWc ligands, including for these drugs themselves, the question of neuroanatomic localization of drug eVects can be addressed more directly than has previously been feasible. This will obviously be of considerable signiWcance for our attempts to localize the neural sites of action of compounds, such as methylphenidate, that have special signiWcance for developmental cognitive and behavioral disorders.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge support from the Wellcome Trust. A. Lee holds a BBSRC studentship. CANTAB is commercially available from CeNeS Cognition, Compass House, Vision Park, Chivers Way, Histon, CB4 4ZR, UK (FAX (0)1223 266467).

iReferencesi

Abas, M. A., Sahakian, B. J. and Levy, R. (1990). Neuropsychological deWcits and CT scan changes in elderly depressives. Psychol. Med., 20, 507±20.

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M. et al. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task.

Neuropsychologia, 34, 515±26.

Beats, B. C., Sahakian, B. J. and Levy, R. (1996). Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed. Psychol. Med., 26, 591±603.

Coull, J. T., Middleton, H. C., Robbins, T. W. and Sahakian, B.J. (1995). Contrasting eVects of clonidine and diazepam on tests of working memory and planning. Psychopharmacology, 120, 311±21.

Coull, J. T., Sahakian, B. J. and Hodges, J. R. (1996a). The ('-2 antagonist, idazoxan, remediates certain attentional and executive dysfunction in patients with dementia of frontal type.

Psychopharmacology, 123, 239±49.

376 A. C. H. Lee et al.

Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J. and Grasby, P. M. (1996b). A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory.

Neuropsychologia, 35, 515±26.

Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Keil, K. and Haxby, J. V. (1996). Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. Cereb. Cortex, 6, 39±49.

Dias, R., Roberts, A. and Robbins, T. W. (1996). Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of aVective and attentional shifts. Nature, 380, 69±72.

Downes, J. J., Roberts, A. C., Sahakian, B. J., Evenden, J. L., Morris, R. G. and Robbins, T. W. (1989). Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: evidence for a speciWc attentional dysfunction.

Neuropsychologia, 27, 1329±43.

Elliott, R. and Dolan, R. J. (1998). The neural response in short-term visual recognition memory for perceptual conjunctions.

Neuroimage, 7, 14±22.

Elliott, R., Sahakian, B. J., McKay, A. P., Herrod, J. J., Robbins, T. W. and Paykel, E. S. (1996). Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the inXuence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychol. Med., 26, 975±89.

Elliott, R., Frith, C. D. and Dolan, R. J. (1997a). DiVerential neural response to positive and negative feedback in planning and guessing tasks. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1395±1404.

Elliott, R., Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D. et al. (1997b). Prefrontal dysfunction in depressed patients performing a complex planning task: a study using positron emission tomography. Psychol. Med.,

27, 931±42.

Elliott, R., Sahakian, B. J., Matthews, K., Bannerjea, A., Rimmer, J. and Robbins, T.W. (1997c). EVects of methylphenidate on spatial working memory and planning in healthy young adults.

Psychopharmacology, 131, 196±206.

Elliott, R., McKenna, P. J., Robbins, T. W. and Sahakian, B. J. (1998). SpeciWc neuropsychological deWcits in schizophrenic patients with preserved intellectual function. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry, 3, 45±70.

Fowler, K. S., Saling, M. M., Conway, E. L., Semple, J. M. and Louis, W. J. (1997). Computerized neuropsychological tests in the early detection of dementia: prospective Wndings. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc., 3, 139±46.

Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and the regulation of behaviour by representational memory. In

Handbook of Physiology, Sect. 1, Vol. 5, The Nervous System, eds. F. Plum and V. Mountcastle, pp. 373±417. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.

Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996). The prefrontal cortex landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B., 351, 1445±53.

Granon, S., Anton, J. L., Dauchot, K. et al. (1998). Human prefrontal cortex activity in planning and working memory using functional MRI. In Proceedings of the 1st FENS Meeting, Berlin, Germany.

Hughes, C., Russell, J. and Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477±92.

Hutton, S. B., Puri, B. K., Duncan, L.-J., Robbins, T. W., Barnes, T. R. E. and Joyce, E. M. (1998). Executive function in Wrst-episode schizophrenia. Psychol. Med., 28, 463±73.

Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Minoshima, S. and Mintun, M. A. (1993). Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature, 363, 623±5.

Joyce, E. M. and Robbins, T. W. (1991). Frontal lobe function in KorsakoV and non-KorsakoV alcoholics: planning and spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia, 29, 709±23.

Klosowska, D. (1976). Relation between ability to program actions and location of brain damage. Pol. Psychol. Bull., 7, 245±55.

Koepp, M. J., Gunn, R. N., Lawrence, A. D. et al. (1998). Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature, 393, 266±8.

Lange, K. W., Sahakian, B. J., Quinn, N. P., Marsden, C. D. and Robbins, T. W. (1995). Comparison of executive and visuospatial memory function in Huntington's disease and dementia of Alzheimer type matched for degree of dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 58, 598±606.

Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Rosser, A. E., Lange, K. W. and Robbins, T. W. (1996). Executive and mnemonic functions in early Huntington's disease. Brain, 119, 1633±45.

Lawrence, A., Sahakian, B. J. and Robbins, T. W. (1998a). Cognitive functions and corticostriatal circuits: insights from Huntington's disease. Trends Cogn. Sci., 2, 379±88.

Lawrence, A. D., Weeks, R. A., Brooks, et al., 1998b). The relationship between striatal dopamine receptor status and cognitive performance in Huntington's disease. Brain, 121, 1343±55.

Luciana, M. and Nelson, C. A. (1998). The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four-to-eight year old children. Neuropsychologia, 36, 273±93.

Milner, B. (1964). Some eVects of frontal lobectomy in man. In The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behaviour, ed. J. M. Warren and K. Akert, pp. 313±31. New York: McGraw Hill.

Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric diVerence and psychological processes. Br. Med. Bull., 27, 272±7.

Milner, B. (1974). Hemispheric specialization: scope and limitations. In The Neurosciences: Third Study Program, eds. F. O. Schmitt and F. G. Worden, pp. 75±89. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Mishkin, M. (1982). A memory system in the monkey. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B., 298, 85±95.

Morris, R. G., Downes, J. J., Evenden, J. L., Sahakian, B. J., Heald, A. and Robbins, T. W. (1988). Planning and spatial working memory in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 51, 757±66.

Morris, R. G., Ahmed, S., Syed, G. M. and Toone, B. K. (1993). Neural correlates of planning ability: frontal lobe activation during the Tower of London test. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1367±78.

Murray, E. (1992). What have ablation studies told us about the neural substrates of stimulus memory? Semin. Neurosci., 8, 13±22.

Olton, D. S. (1982). Spatially organised behaviours of animals: behavioural and neurological studies. In Spatial Abilities, ed. M. Potegal, pp. 325±60. New York: Academic Press.

Owen, A. M. (1997a). Cognitive planning in humans: neuropsy-

The utility of CANTAB in functional imaging

377

 

 

chological, neuroanatomical and neuropharmacological perspectives. Prog. Neurobiol., 59, 4321±450.

Owen, A. M. (1997b). The functional organisation of working memory processes within human lateral frontal cortex: The contribution of functional imaging. Eur. J. Neurosci., 9, 1329±39.

Owen, A. M. and Robbins, T. W. (1993). Comparative neuropsychology of Parkinsonian syndromes. In Mental Dysfunction in Parkinson's Disease, eds. P. Wolters and E. C. Scheltens, pp. 221±42. Amsterdam: Vrieje University Press.

Owen, A. M., Downes, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E. and Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1021±34.

Owen, A. M., Roberts, A. C., Polkey, C. E., Sahakian, B. J. and Robbins, T. W. (1991). Extra-dimensional versus intra-dimen- sional set-shifting performance following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 29, 993±1006.

Owen, A. M., James, M., Leigh, P. N. et al. (1992). Fronto-striatal cognitive deWcits at diVerent stages of Parkinson's disease. Brain,

115, 1727±51.

Owen, A. M., Beksinska, M., James, M. et al. (1993). Visuospatial memory deWcits at diVerent stages of Parkinson's disease

Neuropsychologia, 31, 627±44.

Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Semple, J., Polkey, C. E. and Robbins, T. W. (1995a). Visual-spatial short term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1±24.

Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Summers, B. A., Polkey, C. E. and Robbins, T. W. (1995b). Dopamine-dependent frontostriatal planning deWcits in early Parkinson's disease.

Neuropsychology, 9, 126±40.

Owen, A. M., Doyon, J., Petrides, M. and Evans, A. C. (1996a). Planning and spatial working memory: a positron emission tomography study in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci., 8, 353±64.

Owen, A. M., Evans, A. C. and Petrides, M. (1996b). Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study.

Cereb. Cortex, 6, 31±8.

Owen, A. M., Morris, R. G., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E. and Robbins, T. W. (1996c). Double dissociations of memory and executive functions in working memory tasks following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocam- pectomy in man. Brain, 119, 1597±1615.

Owen, A. M., Doyon, J., Dagher, A., Sadikot, A. and Evans, A. C. (1998). Abnormal basal ganglia outXow in Parkinson's disease identiWed with PET: implications for higher cortical functions.

Brain, 121, 949±65.

Pantelis, C., Barnes, T. R. E.,Nelson, H. E. et al. (1997). Frontal-stri- atal cognitive deWcits in patients with chronic schizophrenia.

Brain, 120, 1823±43.

Passingham, R. E. (1985). Memory of monkeys (Macaca mulatta) with lesions in the prefrontal cortex. Behav. Neurosci., 99, 3±21.

Paulesu, E., Frith, C. D. and Frackowiak, K. S. (1993). The neural

correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature,

362, 342±5.

Petrides, M. (1994). Frontal lobes and working memory: evidence from investigations of the eVects of cortical excisions in nonhuman primates. In Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 9, eds. F. Boller and J. Grafman, pp. 59±82. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Petrides, M. (1996). Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex.

Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B., 351, 1455±61.

Petrides, M. and Milner, B. (1982). DeWcits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal and temporal lobe lesions in man.

Neuropsychologia, 20, 249±62.

Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Evans, A. C. and Meyer, E. (1993a). Dissociations of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 90, 873±7.

Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Evans, A. C. and Meyer, E. (1993b). Functional activation of the human prefrontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 90, 878±82.

Rezai, K., Andreasen, N. C., Allinger, R., Cohen, G., Swaze, V. and O'Leary, D. S. (1993). The neuropsychology of the prefrontal cortex. Arch. Neurol., 50, 636±42.

Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M. et al. (1994a). Cognitive deWcits in progressive supranuclear palsy, Parkinson's disease and multiple system atrophy in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. J. Neurol., Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 57, 79±88.

Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., McInnes, L. and Rabbitt, P. M. (1994b). The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery CANTAB; a factor analytical study in a large number of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia, 5, 266±81.

Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., McInnes, L. and Rabbitt, P. M. (1996). A neural systems approach to the cognitive psychology of aging: studies with CANTAB on a large sample of the normal elderly population. In Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function, ed. P. M. Rabbit, pp. 215±38. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Robbins, T. W., Semple, J., Kumar, R. et al. (1997). EVects of scopolamine on delayed-matching-to-sample and paired associates tests of visual memory and learning in human subjects: comparison with diazepam and implications for dementia.

Psychopharmacology, 134, 95±106.

Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M. et al. (1998). A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging.

J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc., 4, 474±90.

Rogers, R. D., Andrews, T. C., Grasby, P. M., Brooks, D. and Robbins, T. W. (2000). Contrasting cortical and sub-cortical PET activations produced by reversal learning and attentional-set-shifting in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 12, 142±62.

Sahakian, B. M., Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L. et al. (1988). A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimertype dementia and Parkinson's disease. Brain, 111, 695±718.

Sahakian, B. J., Downes, J. J., Eager, S. et al. (1990). Sparing of attentional relative to mnemonic function in a subgroup of patients

378 A. C. H. Lee et al.

with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1197±213.

Sahakian, B. J., Elliott, R., Low, N., Mehta, M., Clark, R. T. and Pozniak, A. L. (1995). Neuropsychological deWcits in tests of executive function in asymptomatic and symptomatic HIV-1 seropositive men. Psychol. Med., 25, 1233±46.

Sahgal, A., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W. et al. (1991). Detection of visual memory and learning deWcits in Alzheimer's disease using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery.

Dementia, 2, 150±8.

Sahgal, A., Galloway, P. H., McKeith, I. G., Edwardson, J. A. and Lloyd, S. (1992). A comparative study of attentional deWcits in senile dementias of the Alzheimer and Lewy body types.

Dementia, 3, 350±4.

Shallice, T. (1982). SpeciWc impairments of planning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B, 298, 199±209.

Talairach, J. and Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar Stereotactic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Stuttgart: Georg. Thieme Verlag.

22

Neurodevelopmental assessment of cognitive function using CANTAB: validation and future goals

Monica Luciana and Charles A. Nelson

Introduction

Despite the existence of test batteries designed to assess general levels of intellectual ability (e.g., the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)), well-validated assessments for the study of brain±behavior relations in children are lacking. The establishment of such relations has proven diYcult, because it has not been feasible to establish links between brain functioning and overt behavior without the use of invasive strategies that cannot typically be justiWed for use in healthy children. The discipline of developmental neuropsychology has entered a new era with the advent of brain imaging techniques that are increasingly less invasive from a methodologic standpoint and aimed at elucidating speciWc structure±function relations (Casey et al., 1995). It is now possible not only to examine the development of speciWc cognitive behaviors but also to do so within a framework that allows direct observation of brain activity in the course of behavior. What is needed within this context is a battery of tests that reliably reXects localized patterns of neural activity in individuals throughout the lifespan. While instruments have been developed for use with age-speciWc populations, there are few, if any, instruments that can be used without changes in task presentation, items, or format to test individuals across a broad range of ages. Hence, comparability of Wndings across age groups is questionable, and the ability to identify developmentally driven changes in functional brain development using such instruments has not been methodologically feasible. The Weld of experimental neuropsychology provides an exception to this general rule in the form of a recently developed neurobehavioral test battery that has been used successfully to pattern trajectories of normal cognitive development in individuals from ages 4 to 90 years. This battery of tests, the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB),

was developed at the University of Cambridge, UK by Barbara Sahakian, Trevor Robbins, and their colleagues (see Chapter 21).

CANTAB

Chapter 21 describes the theoretical rationale and validation of CANTAB. It consists of subtasks that index three behavioral domains: working memory/planning, visual memory, and visual attention. Each task is administered through the use of a touch-screen computer and supervised by clinicians trained in neuropsychologic assessment to measure simple reaction time, discrimination learning, recognition memory for patterns and objects, and working memory skills involving self-guided visual search and planning. All CANTAB subtasks are visually guided and require little, if any, verbal mediation and no verbal responses. While the emphasis on nonverbal processes potentially limits the generalizability of Wndings, it ensures that CANTAB can be used in populations that vary in verbal expertise and/or literacy (Robbins and Sahakian, 1994). Although the CANTAB is currently being used to test individuals with varying levels of language proWciency, the precise limits of language proWciency that are needed to produce valid results have not been determined. Hughes et al. (1994) used selected CANTAB subtasks to study children with autism and mental retardation. Language impairments were present in both populations. In our developmental studies, we have found that healthy children below the age of 4 years are not consistently able to understand task instructions, although this pattern appears to be a consequence of a combination of cognitive immaturity and immature language development. This conclusion is partly based on the observation that older children whom we have tested who are not English

379

380M. Luciana and C. A. Nelson

Table 22.1. Pediatric CANTAB studies: number of participants

 

 

 

Children treated in the

 

 

 

Age group

 

 

neonatal intensive

Probands with

Early deprivation

Temporal lobe

(years)

Normative sample

care unit (NICU)

phenylketonuria (PKU)

(foreign-born adoptees)

lesion

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

4

47

 

 

 

 

 

5

41

 

 

 

1

 

6

64

 

 

 

1

 

7

65

 

11

 

3

 

8

68

 

21

 

 

 

9

34

 

8

 

1

 

10

6

 

 

 

1

 

11

14

 

 

 

 

1

12

10

 

 

 

 

 

13±19

7

 

 

12

 

 

20±29

11

 

 

6

 

 

Total group

368

(189M, 179F)

40 (19M, 21F)

18 (9M, 9F)

7 (3M, 4F)

1 (0M, 1F)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Note: M, male; F female.

proWcient are able to acquire task instructions nonverbally, through visual observation of the task.

Where CANTAB is unique among experimental test batteries is that, in addition to a rigorous theoretical framework that was used to guide subtest selection, its developers have undertaken a comprehensive eVort to validate its neural correlates in adult humans. This validation process has centered around several lines of inquiry: (i) normative studies of behavioral performance in elderly adults, (ii) studies of adults with focal brain lesions, (iii) studies of adults with speciWc neuropathologies, (iv) the eVects of pharmacologic manipulations on task performance, and (v) assessment of brain±behavior correlates using both functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) neuroimaging techniques. These eVorts, which have yielded approximately 80 published reports in the period 1990±99, strongly support the CANTAB as a valid measure for the assessment of frontal, temporal, and/or subcortical/striatal functions in adults. While speciWc aspects of CANTAB performance appear to be aVected by dysfunction in these brain regions, use of the CANTAB in neuroimaging contexts permits not only an examination of the brain regions that mediate speciWc cognitive processes but also an examination of the large-scale neural networks that support cognition (Chapter 21).

Use of the CANTAB in pediatric populations

In contrast to its increasingly widespread use in adult samples, the use of CANTAB with pediatric populations has been quite limited. Prior to 1995, only a single study reported on the use of the CANTAB in impaired children

(Hughes et al., 1994). In 1995, the MacArthur Research Network on Psychopathology and Development began a systematic validation of the CANTAB as an assessment tool for children. As part of this project, we are in the process of collecting normative data from healthy children between the ages of 4 and 18 years (see Luciana and Nelson, 1998), as well as data from several clinical populations (Luciana et al., 1999; M. Luciana, J. Sullivan and C. A. Nelson, unpublished data). Table 22.1 summarizes our sample characteristics to date. Our aim, through this research endeavor, is to generate data on tyically and atypically developing children using a behavioral battery with increasingly wellestablished neural correlates in adults to determine whether these same correlates exist in children.

The CANTAB appears to be particularly amenable to the study of children. Although the use of computerized assessment in clinical evaluation has been controversial, the computerized format readily lends itself to rapport building, even among atypically developing children. The method of test administration is highly standardized, and variations in Wndings owing to experimenter error are negligible. Although children must possess enough verbal skill to understand task instructions, there are no additional demands for verbally mediated information processing in the course of testing.

CANTAB and the developmental assessment of frontal lobe function

Because of its role in mediating high-level cognition including working memory and planning functions, the prefrontal

CANTAB: validation and future goals

381

 

 

cortex is being widely studied from a developmental standpoint (Diamond and Goldman-Rakic, 1989; Diamond, 1990; Welsh et al., 1991; Huttenlocher and Dabhholker, 1997). Based on neurophysiologic studies in humans and primates, it has been suggested that the prefrontal cortex is not physiologically mature in the human until some time point between adolescence and early adulthood. For instance, PET studies have demonstrated that while metabolic activity in the temporal lobes increases within the Wrst 3 postnatal months, similar rates of glucose utilization in the prefrontal cortex are not reached until several months later (Chugani, 1994; Chugani and Phelps, 1986). The process of myelination does not appear to be complete until adolescence, as indicated by autopsy studies (Huttenlocher, 1994; Huttenlocher and Dabhholker, 1997). While synaptogenesis, the formation of functional connections between cells, peaks in the frontal cortex during early childhood, the selective pruning of unnecessary connections follows a more protracted course; synaptic density within the frontal cortex appears to decline well into early adulthood (Huttenlocher, 1994; Huttenlocher and Dabhholker, 1997).

Whether these neural changes are directly correlated with the functional development of the frontal lobe has not been studied in humans. What has been done is to identify behavioral functions that are disrupted by frontal lobe lesions and, hence, mediated by intact frontal functioning. The most consistently employed measure of prefrontal function in experimental studies has been the delayed response task (Jacobsen, 1936), formally similar to Piaget's A-not-B task. Goldman-Rakic and colleagues (GoldmanRakic, 1987) have convincingly demonstrated that spatial delayed response performance (also referred to as spatial working memory) is mediated by the dorsolateral region of the prefrontal cortex in nonhuman primates. Additionally, there is a correspondence between the development of spatial working memory skills in the monkey and the ontogenetic development of the same abilities in human infants (Diamond and Goldman-Rakic, 1989). That the prefrontal cortex mediates aspects of working memory performance in adults is supported by a plethora of studies (see Damasio and Anderson (1994) and Goldman-Rakic (1987) for reviews). The accurate sensorimotor integration of representational traces linking past events with future goals is core to deWnitions of working memory (GoldmanRakic, 1987; Fuster, 1995). Hence, empirical measures that have been used to assess working memory functions in adult humans are relatively complicated in nature and include not only the delayed response task but also temporal judgments (Smith and Milner, 1988), self-ordered searching tasks (Petrides and Milner, 1982), and lookahead planning tasks (Shallice, 1982). While young chil-

dren, like adults with frontal lobe lesions, might demonstrate the component processes necessary for working memory ± that is, intact recognition memory, sensory perception, and motor skills ± they may lack the cognitive resources to organize, monitor, and/or strategize their behavioral actions to integrate the present environmental context with future outcomes (Damasio, 1994; Damasio and Anderson, 1994; Luciana and Nelson, 1998). Indeed, it may not be until the age of at least 12 years that children exemplify adult levels of ªmetacognitionº: the type of attentional monitoring that permits self-evaluation of ongoing sequences of behavior in an executive manner (Flavell et al., 1966; Passler et al., 1985).

Our research is aimed at examining the emergence of executive functions in normal children between the ages of 4 and 18 years and validating the utility of CANTAB as a measure of frontal lobe function in children. The primary measures of frontal lobe function within the CANTAB battery are (i) spatial working memory, a self-guided search task; (ii) the Tower of London, a test of planning and behavioral inhibition; and (iii) the intradimensional/extradimensional set-shifting task, which measures the ability to shift cognitive response sets both within and across categories. CANTAB measures temporal lobe recognition memory functions through delayed-match-to-sample (DMTS) recognition memory tasks. Functional imaging using PET and fMRI in adults have revealed the neural circuitry underlying performance on variations of these tasks.

CANTAB assessment of frontal lobe function

Spatial working memory

The spatial working memory test is a self-ordered searching task (Petrides and Milner, 1982) that measures working memory for spatial stimuli and requires the subject to use mnemonic information to work toward a goal. On each trial of this task, a number of colored squares are displayed on the screen (Fig. 22.1). The child is told that tokens are hidden inside the colored squares. To Wnd a token, the child must touch the colored squares one at a time to ªopenº them. If a square contains a token, the child must move it to another area of the screen to ªput it awayº. Each colored square will contain only one token at some point in the course of a trial. Hence, the rule for the task is that once a token is found inside a colored square, there will never be another token inside that same square. In order to perform the task most eYciently without searching repeatedly in previously targeted locations, the child must remember where s/he has searched and found a token. Returning to

382 M. Luciana and C. A. Nelson

Fig. 22.1. The spatial working memory task. Color names are presented for clarity but are not part of the actual stimulus display. On each trial, colored squares are presented on the screen. The subject must touch a colored square to ªopenº it. When a blue token is found inside a square, the subject must place it in the black column at the right of the screen. The square then ªclosesº (i.e., returns to its original color). When the subject begins to search for other tokens, s/he must ignore squares where tokens have been found. If the subject returns to search a location where a token has been found, s/he has made a ªforgetting errorº.

an ªemptyº box where a token has already been found during a particular search constitutes a ªforgettingº error.

The order in which the subject searches the colored squares is self-determined. The number of colored squares starts at two. The subject ultimately completes four trials each with 2, 3, 4, 6, and 8 items.

It is possible to reduce the memory load for a given trial by searching strategically for the tokens. One strategy that has been deWned as eVective is to follow a predetermined search sequence, beginning with a particular square and then, once a token is found, returning to that same starting point when initiating the next search (Owen et al., 1990; Fray et al., 1996). The extent to which this repetitive search strategy is used is estimated from the number of searches that start with the same location, within each of the 6-item and 8-item searches. A high score (many searches starting

with diVerent locations) indicates low use of this strategy, whereas a low score (many searches starting with the same location) indicates more consistent use of this strategy.

Based on PET data, this task appears to activate both the dorsal and ventral prefrontal regions (Owen et al., 1996a). Additionally, adult neurosurgical patients with frontal lobe lesions demonstrate distinct patterns of performance, namely high numbers of mnemonic errors as well as deWcient use of strategy in guiding their performance (Owen et al., 1990, 1996c). These errors occur at all levels of task diYculty and appear to be restricted to working memory within the spatial (versus verbal) domain (Owen et al., 1996c). In contrast, patients with damage to the temporal lobe demonstrate a high number of mnemonic errors but in the context of normal strategy use. Their mnemonic errors appear to occur only at the most diYcult levels of the

CANTAB: validation and future goals

383

 

 

Fig. 22.2. The intradimensional/extradimensional set-shifting task. The subject views two patterns on the screen and must select the one that is ªcorrectº. If the subject selects the correct pattern, it turns green, providing positive reinforcement to the subject. The Wrst two stages of the task involve (I) discriminating between two lined patterns and then (II) reversing one's response to the other pattern when selection of the Wrst one is no longer reinforced. On subsequent stages of the task (III, IV, and V), a second shaped pattern is added to the lined patterns. The subject is reinforced for continuing to respond to lined patterns while ignoring the ªshapeº dimension. The Wrst demand for a shift occurs at stage VI. The subject views new exemplars of lines and shaped Wgures but is still reinforced for responding to lines at stages VI and VII (intradimensional shift and reversal, respectively). At stages VIII and IX, the subject is reinforced for switching his/her responses from ªlineº to ªshapeº (the extradimensional shift and reversal). Note, stage IV is intentionally omitted from the Wgure because it does not add signiWcant graphical information.

task (Owen et al., 1996c). Using a modiWed version of the spatial working memory task, Owen and colleagues (1996a) have demonstrated that when normal control subjects perform the most diYcult task items in the course of PET neuroimaging, signiWcant changes in regional cerebral blood Xow (rCBF) are evident in the right mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as well as bilaterally in the ventrolateral frontal region. Additionally, the right hippocampus is activated. These Wndings have led Owen and colleagues (1996a, p. 1611) to conclude that ªspatial working memory involves a network of interconnected and functionally related cortical and subcortical areas including, at the very least, the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampusº, a viewpoint that is consistent with what has been found in nonhuman primates (Goldman-Rakic, 1987).

Intradimensional/extradimensional set-shifting

One of the most popularly used measures for the assessment of frontal lobe dysfunction is the Wisconsin Card Sort Test (WCST; Milner, 1964). It is a well-established phenomenon that patients with frontal lobe dysfunction, as well as patients with schizophrenia, demonstrate a high number of

errors on this test, and perseverative errors in particular. Since the WCST requires the examinee to shift response set among three dimensions (color, shape, and number) in response to verbal feedback (correct or incorrect), deWcient performance has been attributed to the inability to shift response set between conceptual categories. However, the precise nature of the cognitive deWcit underlying task failure has yet to be determined, because the task actually requires several abilities, including the ability to discriminate relative stimulus cues perceptually, the ability to respond to feedback about correct versus incorrect performance, the ability to attend selectively to one response set when another is present, and the ability to shift between sets. Through the intradimensional/extradimensional set-shift task, the CANTAB provides a method to assess these separate components of cognitive function within the same task.

This task measures discrimination and reversal learning under conditions in which the subject is required to shift attention to changing patterns of visual stimuli. A full description can be found in Downes et al. (1989). BrieXy, this task progresses along a series of stages of increasing diYculty (Fig. 22.2). In the Wrst stage (termed the ªsimple discriminationº stage), the child is required to learn a